Uppsala University: Socioeconomic deprivation modifies genetic influence on higher education
A comprehensive study from Uppsala University demonstrates that socioeconomic deprivation modifies genetic effects on higher education and abstract reasoning. The paper illustrates how genes play a greater role in educational attainment in more socioeconomically deprived regions of the United Kingdom. The study was recently published in the American Journal of Psychiatry.
Education is an important factor in an individual’s life and strongly linked to economic outcomes and quality of life. The likelihood of completing higher education is partly determined by genetic factors. Common genetic variants have previously been estimated to contribute 11–13% of the variance in whether an individual completes higher education. This heritability is an estimate of how much a trait is influenced by genes.
In the current study, scientists at Uppsala University assessed the heritability of higher educational attainment and performance on a verbal and numerical reasoning test. Heritability was compared across the socioeconomic spectrum in more than 350,000 participants in the UK Biobank cohort.
The researchers found that common genetic variants contributed 13% of the variance in the least socioeconomically deprived, but almost 26% in the most deprived. “We were surprised to find that the heritability was higher in participants from more socioeconomically deprived regions of the UK. This is the opposite of what previous twin studies have found,” says the lead author of the study, Dr Mathias Rask-Andersen. This type of difference in heritability across the socioeconomic spectrum is likely to represent an interaction between genes and the environment, where socioeconomic deprivation modifies genetic effects.
Previous studies in the field have mainly been performed in small twin cohorts from the United States. According to Dr Rask-Andersen, the contrasting results of the current study indicate that national differences may influence genetic effects. For instance, differences in access to and quality of education across the socioeconomic spectrum may play an important role, as well as welfare support systems and healthcare.
The current study illustrates how the environment determines how an individual’s genetic makeup is propagated. “More and more studies demonstrate that environmental factors influence genetic effects,” explains Dr Åsa Johansson, group leader at the Department of Immunology, Genetics and Pathology at Uppsala University. “We have previously seen similar gene-environment interactions for other traits, such as body mass index, and it is likely that interactions also exist for many human diseases.”
In the long run, gene-environment interactions offer an additional layer of information on the contribution of genetics to human traits and diseases. Identifying these interactions can potentially lead to more precise forms of interventions, not only for preventing and treating disease, but also for increasing the likelihood of successful educational outcomes.
The results from this study also have implications for notions of equality of outcome and opportunity. “The larger influence of genetic factors in more socioeconomically deprived regions could reflect greater challenges for higher education in these regions,” says Dr Rask-Andersen. “Someone who is more vulnerable due to genetic factors would then face even greater challenges in a more socioeconomically deprived region.”